This work may not be copied, distributed, displayed, published, reproduced, transmitted, modified, posted, sold, licensed, or used for commercial purposes. By downloading this file, you are agreeing to the publisher’s Terms & Conditions.

Understanding the Lifetime Course of Schizophrenia

The Longitudinal Course of Cognitive Impairment in Schizophrenia: An Examination of Data From Premorbid Through Posttreatment Phases of Illness

Richard S. E. Keefe, PhD

Published: June 5, 2014

The Longitudinal Course of Cognitive Impairment in Schizophrenia: An Examination of Data From Premorbid Through Posttreatment Phases of Illness

Cognitive impairment is a core feature of schizophrenia that is present across the course of the illness. However, due to complexities of studying cognitive decline in patients prior to the onset of illness, the longitudinal course is not fully understood. The cognitive effects in patients with schizophrenia are robust, with a 1.5 to 2.5 standard deviation gap between patients and healthy controls on composite scores. People with schizophrenia manifest a prior history of cognitive impairment in the premorbid phases of the illness. Examination of school records suggests that children who will eventually develop schizophrenia begin school at a level of functioning that is a full grade behind their peers, with the gap increasing by the time they finish high school. Epidemiologic work suggests that there are both static cognitive impairments and developmental lags in these patients during childhood, well before the illness is fully manifest. Although there was initial promise of improved cognitive function with second-generation antipsychotic treatment, more recent studies have suggested no differences among antipsychotics, with the initial appearance of improvement very likely attributable to practice effects, inappropriate medication dosing, and poor study design. Two large, prominent studies evaluating first- and second-generation antipsychotics suggested that, although there was slight to modest improvement in cognitive function for all treatments, there were no differences among medications, regardless of the generation of the agents. In summary, patients who develop schizophrenia, on average, demonstrate cognitive impairment beginning as early as the first grade, with deterioration seen across school years. Further, these patients had substantial cognitive deficits after the initiation of psychosis. Finally, while antipsychotic treatment improves symptoms, antipsychotics have little impact on cognition, and there appear to be no differences in the degree of cognitive improvement between first- and second-generation agents.

(J Clin Psychiatry 2014;75[suppl 2]:8-13)

Corresponding author: Richard S. E. Keefe, PhD, Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences and Psychology, Duke University Medical Center, Durham, NC 27710 ([email protected]).

Cognitive impairment is widely considered a core feature of schizophrenia. The central reason for this consensus stems from the presence of these deficits at various time points in the lifetime of patients with the illness. However, the longitudinal course of cognitive dysfunction in patients with schizophrenia is not clearly understood. The key questions that need to be answered include (1) When does cognitive dysfunction begin? (2) Does it predate the onset of psychosis? (3) Is there further decline in function after the onset of psychosis and what is the course of the decline? and (4) Do factors such as clinical symptoms affect cognitive performance?

Cognitive dysfunction regularly begins in adolescence in those that later develop schizophrenia.1-7 The evidence examining the course of deficits after illness onset is less consistent. A pattern of decline in cognitive function from before to after the onset of psychotic symptoms is evident.8-13 However, cognitive function improvement after the emergence of initial psychosis has also been demonstrated,9,14-16 suggesting a potential enigma regarding the longitudinal course of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Further, there is no clear evidence of progressive decline across time in the cognitive function of prospectively analyzed first-episode patients, suggesting stability and possible mild improvement.17-20

Cross-sectional cognitive data suggest that virtually every aspect of cognition (eg, attention, memory, and language) is impaired in schizophrenia patients on average.21-28 However, not all patients with schizophrenia show the same pattern or degree of impairment. While the majority of patients demonstrate some type of impairment, some patients fall within the normal range (± 1 standard deviation of the healthy population mean). However, even this small percentage of patients in the normal range may have significant decline from premorbid levels of cognitive function.29

A consistent relationship between cognitive deficits and negative symptoms, psychotic symptoms, and disorganized symptoms has been seen across a variety of studies. The negative symptom dimension appears to have the strongest relationship with cognitive performance. Negative symptoms are correlated with deficits in measures of generalized brain dysfunction,30 visual and motor processing,24,25,31,32 long-term memory,33,34 and attentional processes.34-38 Positive symptoms on the other hand have an inconsistent relationship with cognitive function. Although auditory processing deficits,35 verbal memory impairments24,25 and auditory distractibility37 were correlated with positive symptoms in small samples, more recent data from a large sample suggest that these 2 domains are largely orthogonal to one another.29

The aim of this review is to examine some of the more recent studies evaluating the course of cognitive impairment in order to determine our current understanding. Additionally, the gaps in the literature on the longitudinal course of schizophrenia will be discussed.

A SAMPLE OF CROSS-SECTIONAL COGNITIVE DEFICITS IN PATIENTS With SCHIZOPHRENIA

The first reviewed data are from the cognitive test battery derived from the Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia (MATRICS) project. The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB) was designed to evaluate the cognitive effects of treatment in schizophrenia patients and allowed an in-depth examination of the cross-sectional baseline differences between patients with schizophrenia and healthy adults.39 Patients with schizophrenia were approximately 2.5 standard deviations below the healthy control mean on the composite score, with deficits seen across all individual scores (Figure 1).40 To put the severity of this impairment into perspective, a difference of 2.5 standard deviations from the healthy control mean on measures of intellectual functioning such as IQ scores would be < 70, consistent with a diagnosis of mental retardation. These results are consistent with other studies indicating a similar range of impairment.41,42

Figure 1

Click figure to enlarge

The severity and profile of cognitive impairment have also been evaluated at the initial onset of schizophrenia. In one such study by Bilder et al,43 patients exhibited global cognitive deficits of 1.5 standard deviations relative to a healthy control group at the first psychotic episode. Memory was the most impaired specific domain, with additional deficits in motor and executive function. The data from this and other similar studies make clear that the level of cognitive impairment at the first episode is severe.

Although no single study has evaluated cognitive impairment from adolescence through mortality, an amalgamation of cross-sectional studies and short-term longitudinal studies allows us to attempt to infer the longitudinal pattern across time.44 As reviewed below, a wealth of evidence suggests that, on average, individuals in the prodromal period prior to psychosis onset have relatively mild cognitive impairment, with the mean level of cognitive function about 0.5 standard deviation below the general population mean. However, what is the course of cognitive impairment from prodrome to psychosis onset? Very few studies have been able to examine this question, and it remains unclear whether these patients have severe cognitive impairment immediately prior to the onset of psychosis or whether this cognitive impairment emerges alongside the onset of psychosis. After initial treatment with antipsychotic medication in first-episode patients, it appears that there is a small cognitive improvement that is likely due to the reduction of positive symptoms such as hallucinations and delusions (Figure 2).44 However, these data do not suggest much further improvement beyond the initial 1-2 years of treatment.

Figure 2

Click figure to enlarge

One of the important questions that arises in the consideration of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia is "Which patients are affected?" Since some patients will perform up to healthy control means on cognitive tests, there is some question as to whether they have cognitive impairment. It is difficult to determine whether these individuals’ cognitive functioning has been unaffected by schizophrenia, or whether they would have been performing in the higher reaches of the general population if they had not developed the illness. However, one way of determining whether individuals have current cognitive functioning that is worse than expected is to compare them to the predictions made of their current cognition based on antecedent factors such as parental education. One such analysis suggested that there is a strong relationship between maternal education and current cognitive function, with about 50% of individuals performing above expectations and 50% performing below expectations. However, 98% of the patients with schizophrenia in this study performed below the expectations set by their mothers’ education.45 These data suggest that almost all patients with schizophrenia have some worsening of cognitive functioning compared to cognitive performance that would be expected if they did not have the illness.

EXAMINATION OF PREMORBID AND LONGITUDINAL SYMPTOMS IN PATIENTS WHO LATER DEVELOP SCHIZOPHRENIA

A number of studies have reported a prior history of cognitive impairment in those who eventually develop schizophrenia.46 Draftees into the Israeli army who were later diagnosed with schizophrenia performed 0.5 standard deviation worse than the draftees who did not develop psychiatric illness,47 and studies examining school records have suggested that children who will develop schizophrenia later in life on average start out 1 grade level behind their peers and, by the time they finish high school, are even further behind.48 These data suggest that there are important cognitive deficits in at least a small percentage of children who will eventually develop schizophrenia; however, it is not practical to use this approach to identify those individuals because only a minority of patients can be successfully predicted, and the false positive and false negative rates are very high.

Premorbid academic performance was used in a retrospective longitudinal study of schizophrenia patients to determine if their performance differed from that of their healthy peers.49 Standardized test scores were lower than average at grades 4 and 8, but not significantly different. However, a significant drop in scores occurred between grades 8 and 11, a timeframe corresponding to the onset of puberty. Further, these declines predicted those who subsequently developed schizophrenia. Although these deficits may be striking when looking at data from the average performance of the children who developed schizophrenia, it is likely that the day-to-day experience with individual adolescents is not noteworthy in the classroom. Teachers often monitor their children within each school year, and the main concerns that prompt attention and action are disciplinary issues, serious academic difficulty, and behavioral problems. Thus, the subtle cognitive decline identified between the 3-year interval from grades 8 and 11 may not be recognized in real world educational systems.

Differences in academic performance were demonstrated in a similar premorbid study, with a full grade level difference seen in first grade.48 The early grade gap was expanded to 1.8 grades by the end of high school, indicating that those who later developed schizophrenia exhibited an immediate deficit at the onset of school, with impairment slowly increasing across time to nearly 2 full grades. Conclusions from these data were that schizophrenia is marked by substantial cognitive deficits upon entry into school. Additionally, subtle declines occurred across time, preceding the overt onset of psychotic symptoms, with further impairment at the initial episode of illness.

A community study from New Zealand assessed most of the approximately 1,000 residents (91%) from birth, with a series of assessments initiated at 3 years of age extending out 30 years.13 Thirty-five adults went on to develop schizophrenia, allowing retrospective examination of premorbid symptoms. These data allowed evaluation of the 3 potential theories of the course of cognitive decline: (1) the developmental deterioration hypothesis that predicts steady decline; (2) the developmental deficit hypothesis that predicts consistent but stable cognitive impairment; and (3) the developmental lag hypothesis that predicts growth in cognitive abilities, but growth that lags behind that of healthy individuals. There were 2 interrelated developmental processes evident from childhood to early adolescence (ages 7-13 years). Static cognitive impairments were found on measures assessing verbal and visual knowledge acquisition, reasoning, and conceptualization in the children who developed schizophrenia as adults (Figure 3). Impairments emerged early and remained stable. In addition to the static deficits, these children exhibited developmental lags in processing speed, attention, visual-spatial problem solving ability, and working memory. Accordingly, these data provided evidence for both the cognitive developmental deficit and the developmental lag theories.13

Figure 3

Click figure to enlarge

THE COURSE OF COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT POSTTREATMENT: WHAT IS THE EVIDENCE FROM RECENT LARGE TRIALS?

As documented previously, cognitive deficits are pervasive in schizophrenia and are an important target for treatment. Although early studies raised promise that second-generation antipsychotics may provide cognitive benefit beyond first-generation antipsychotics,50,51 large multisite trials have suggested that there are no differences among antipsychotics and that the original apparent benefit is attributable to practice effects, inappropriate medication dosing, and poor study design.53 The impact on cognitive function of haloperidol and several second-generation antipsychotics was assessed in patients with schizophrenia after the first episode of psychosis.52 The study design provided more generalizability because of the broad eligibility criteria. Composite cognitive scores improved modestly across the 6-month period in the treatment groups (Figure 4),52 and there were no significant treatment differences between antipsychotics. The authors concluded that there were minimal to no differences in cognitive improvement across the 5 antipsychotics.

Figure 4

Click figure to enlarge

The Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE) study was designed to compare cognitive performance between perphenazine (first-generation antipsychotic), olanzapine, quetiapine, and risperidone (second-generation drugs).54 Cognitive function was slightly improved in all groups after 2, 6, and 18 months of treatment; however, there were no significant differences in cognitive scores between the groups. The overall conclusion from CATIE was that the second-generation agents appeared to provide no cognitive advantage compared with perphenazine, and that the small cognitive benefit reported in all groups was consistent with a small practice effect.55

CONCLUSION

In summary, cognitive impairment is a core feature of schizophrenia. However, due to complexities of studying cognitive decline in patients prior to the onset of illness, the longitudinal course is not fully understood. Substantial cognitive deficits, ranging from 1.5 to 2.5 standard deviations below healthy controls, have been reported in numerous studies. Further, the functional ability of patients, even those with cognitive performance within the normal range, is affected. Premorbid academic performance in longitudinal studies suggests that, on average, cognitive deficits are present on entry into school.48,49 Additional deficits may accumulate or worsen across time prior to the onset of psychotic symptoms. Finally, while antipsychotic treatment improves symptoms, which may have some secondary effect of improving cognitive performance in the very early stages of psychosis, there are no differences in the degree of cognitive improvement between first- and second-generation agents.53,54

Drug names: haloperidol (Haldol and others), olanzapine (Zyprexa), quetiapine (Seroquel), risperidone (Risperdal and others).

Author affiliation: Duke University Medical Center, Durham, North Carolina.

Potential conflicts of interest: Dr Keefe received a fee for service from Otsuka America Pharmaceutical, Inc., and Lundbeck for participation in the meeting and preparation of this manuscript. Currently or in the past 12 months, he has received investigator-initiated research funding support from the Department of Veterans Affairs, Feinstein Institute for Medical Research, National Institute of Mental Health, Research Foundation for Mental Hygiene, Inc, and the Singapore National Medical Research Council; has received honoraria, served as a consultant, or served as an advisory board member for Abbvie, Akebia, Amgen, Asubio, AviNeuro/ChemRar, BiolineRx, Biomarin, Boehringer-Ingelheim, EnVivo, GW Pharmaceuticals, Lundbeck, Merck, Mitsubishi, Novartis, Otsuka America Pharmaceutical, Inc., Roche, Shire, Takeda, and Targacept; receives royalties from the BACS testing battery, the MATRICS Battery (BACS Symbol Coding), and the Virtual Reality Functional Capacity Assessment Tool (VRFCAT), and is also a shareholder in NeuroCog Trials, Inc, and Sengenix.

Acknowledgments: This article is derived from a roundtable meeting titled "Understanding the lifetime course of schizophrenia: a longitudinal perspective on neurobiology to promote better outcomes and recovery," which was held October 15, 2013. Editorial assistance in developing the manuscript was provided by Healthcare Global Village.

Funding/support: The meeting, manuscript preparation, and dissemination of the supplement were sponsored by Otsuka America Pharmaceutical, Inc., and Lundbeck.

REFERENCES

1. Pollack M, Woerner M, Klein DF. A comparison of childhood characteristics of schizophrenics, personality disorders, and their siblings. In: Roff M, Ricks DF, eds. Life History Research in Psychopathology. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press; 1970:208-225.

2. Parnas J, Schulsinger F, Schulsinger H, et al. Behavioral precursors of schizophrenia spectrum: a prospective study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1982;39(6):658-664. PubMed doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1982.04290060020005

3. Aylward E, Walker E, Bettes B. Intelligence in schizophrenia: meta-analysis of the research. Schizophr Bull. 1984;10(3):430-459. PubMed doi:10.1093/schbul/10.3.430

4. Mirsky AF, Silberman EK, Latz A, et al. Adult outcomes of high-risk children: differential effects of town and kibbutz rearing. Schizophr Bull. 1985;11(1):150-154. PubMed doi:10.1093/schbul/11.1.150

5. Parnas J, Schulsinger H. Continuity of formal thought disorder from childhood to adulthood in a high-risk sample. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1986;74(3):246-251. PubMed doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1986.tb06240.x

6. Done DJ, Crow TJ, Johnstone EC, et al. Childhood antecedents of schizophrenia and affective illness: social adjustment at ages 7 and 11. BMJ. 1994;309(6956):699-703. PubMed doi:10.1136/bmj.309.6956.699

7. Jones P, Rodgers B, Murray R, et al. Child development risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. Lancet. 1994;344(8934):1398-1402. PubMed doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(94)90569-X

8. Payne R. Cognitive abnormalities. In: Eysenck HJ, ed. Handbook of Abnormal Psychology. New York, NY: Basic Books; 1961:193-261.

9. Schwartzman AE, Douglas VI, Muir WR. Intellectual loss in schizophrenia. II. Can J Psychol. 1962;16(2):161-168. PubMed doi:10.1037/h0083240

10. Smith A. Mental deterioration in chronic schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1964;139(5):479-487. PubMed doi:10.1097/00005053-196411000-00008

11. Silverberg-Shalev R, Gordon HW, Bentin S, et al. Selective language deterioration in chronic schizophrenia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1981;44(6):547-551. PubMed doi:10.1136/jnnp.44.6.547

12. Meier MH, Shalev I, Moffitt TE, et al. Microvascular abnormality in schizophrenia as shown by retinal imaging. Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170(12):1451-1459. PubMed doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13020234

13. Reichenberg A, Caspi A, Harrington H, et al. Static and dynamic cognitive deficits in childhood preceding adult schizophrenia: a 30-year study. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(2):160-169. PubMed doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09040574

14. Haywood HC, Moelis I. Effect of symptom change on intellectual function in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Soc Psychol. 1963;67(1):76-78. PubMed doi:10.1037/h0046251

15. Klonoff H, Fibiger CH, Hutton GH. Neuropsychological patterns in chronic schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1970;150(4):291-300. PubMed doi:10.1097/00005053-197004000-00004

16. Heaton RK, Crowley TJ. Effects of psychiatric disorders and their somatic treatments on neuropsychological test results. In: Felskov SB, Boll TJ, eds. Handbook of Clinical Neuropsychology. Vol 1. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons; 1981:481-525.

17. Nopoulos P, Flashman L, Flaum M, et al. Stability of cognitive functioning early in the course of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 1994;14(1):29-37. PubMed doi:10.1016/0920-9964(94)90006-X

18. DeLisi LE, Tew W, Xie S, et al. A prospective follow-up study of brain morphology and cognition in first-episode schizophrenic patients: preliminary findings. Biol Psychiatry. 1995;38(6):349-360. PubMed doi:10.1016/0006-3223(94)00376-E

19. Sweeney JA, Haas GL, Keilp JG, et al. Evaluation of the stability of neuropsychological functioning after acute episodes of schizophrenia: one-year followup study. Psychiatry Res. 1991;38(1):63-76. PubMed doi:10.1016/0165-1781(91)90053-R

20. Censits DM, Ragland JD, Gur RC, et al. Neuropsychological evidence supporting a neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia: a longitudinal study. Schizophr Res. 1997;24(3):289-298. PubMed doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(96)00091-6

21. Hoff AL, Riordan H, O’ Donnell DW, et al. Neuropsychological functioning of first-episode schizophreniform patients. Am J Psychiatry. 1992;149(7):898-903. PubMed

22. Calev A, Venables PH, Monk AF. Evidence for distinct verbal memory pathologies in severely and mildly disturbed schizophrenics. Schizophr Bull. 1983;9(2):247-264. PubMed doi:10.1093/schbul/9.2.247

23. Levin S, Yurgelun-Todd D, Craft S. Contributions of clinical neuropsychology to the study of schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 1989;98(4):341-356. PubMed doi:10.1037/0021-843X.98.4.341

24. Green M, Walker E. Neuropsychological performance and positive and negative symptoms in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 1985;94(4):460-469. PubMed doi:10.1037/0021-843X.94.4.460

25. Green M, Walker E. Attentional performance in positive- and negative-symptom schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1986;174(4):208-213. PubMed doi:10.1097/00005053-198604000-00003

26. Saykin AJ, Gur RC, Gur RE, et al. Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia: selective impairment in memory and learning. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48(7):618-624. PubMed doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810310036007

27. Goldberg TE, Hyde TM, Kleinman JE, et al. Course of schizophrenia: neuropsychological evidence for a static encephalopathy. Schizophr Bull. 1993;19(4):797-804. PubMed doi:10.1093/schbul/19.4.797

28. Nuechterlein KH, Dawson ME. Information processing and attentional functioning in the developmental course of schizophrenic disorders. Schizophr Bull. 1984;10(2):160-203. PubMed doi:10.1093/schbul/10.2.160

29. Keefe RS, Bilder RM, Harvey PD, et al. Baseline neurocognitive deficits in the CATIE schizophrenia trial. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31(9):2033-2046. PubMed doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301072

30. Hammer MA, Katsanis J, Iacono WG. The relationship between negative symptoms and neuropsychological performance. Biol Psychiatry. 1995;37(11):828-830. PubMed doi:10.1016/0006-3223(95)00040-N

31. Strauss ME. Relations of symptoms to cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1993;19(2):215-231. PubMed doi:10.1093/schbul/19.2.215

32. Cuesta MJ, Peralta V. Cognitive disorders in the positive, negative, and disorganization syndromes of schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 1995;58(3):227-235. PubMed doi:10.1016/0165-1781(95)02712-6

33. Liddle PF. Schizophrenic syndromes, cognitive performance and neurological dysfunction. Psychol Med. 1987;17(1):49-57. PubMed doi:10.1017/S0033291700012976

34. Braff DL. Sensory input deficits and negative symptoms in schizophrenic patients. Am J Psychiatry. 1989;146(8):1006-1011. PubMed

35. Strauss ME, Buchanan RW, Hale J. Relations between attentional deficits and clinical symptoms in schizophrenic outpatients. Psychiatry Res. 1993;47(3):205-213. PubMed doi:10.1016/0165-1781(93)90079-V

36. Nuechterlein KH, Edell WS, Norris M, et al. Attentional vulnerability indicators, thought disorder, and negative symptoms. Schizophr Bull. 1986;12(3):408-426. PubMed doi:10.1093/schbul/12.3.408

37. Cornblatt BA, Lenzenweger MF, Dworkin RH, et al. Positive and negative schizophrenic symptoms, attention, and information processing. Schizophr Bull. 1985;11(3):397-408. PubMed doi:10.1093/schbul/11.3.397

38. Green M, Walker E. Susceptibility to backward masking in schizophrenic patients with positive or negative symptoms. Am J Psychiatry. 1984;141(10):1273-1275. PubMed

39. Nuechterlein KH, Green MF, Kern RS, et al. The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, part 1: test selection, reliability, and validity. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165(2):203-213. PubMed doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07010042

40. Keefe RSE, Fox KH, Harvey PD, et al. Characteristics of the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery in a 29-site antipsychotic schizophrenia clinical trial. Schizophr Res. 2011;125(2-3):161-168. PubMed doi:10.1016/j.schres.2010.09.015

41. August SM, Kiwanuka JN, McMahon RP, et al. The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB): clinical and cognitive correlates. Schizophr Res. 2012;134(1):76-82. PubMed doi:10.1016/j.schres.2011.10.015

42. Umbricht D, Keefe RS, Murray S, et al. A randomized, placebo-controlled study investigating the nicotinic α7 agonist, RG3487, for cognitive deficits in schizophrenia [published online ahead of print January 27, 2014]. Neuropsychopharmacology. PubMed doi:10.1038/npp.2014.17

43. Bilder RM, Goldman RS, Robinson D, et al. Neuropsychology of first-episode schizophrenia: initial characterization and clinical correlates. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157(4):549-559. PubMed doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.4.549

44. Keefe RSE, Eesley CE. Neurocognition of schizophrenia. In: Sadock BJ, Sadock VA, Ruiz P, eds. Kaplan and Sadock Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry; Philadelphia, PA: Lippincot Williams & Wilkins; 2009:1531-1541.

45. Keefe RSE, Eesley CE, Poe MP. Defining a cognitive function decrement in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(6):688-691. PubMed doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.003

46. Seidman LJ, Giuliano AJ, Meyer EC, et al; North American Prodrome Longitudinal Study (NAPLS) Group. Neuropsychology of the prodrome to psychosis in the NAPLS consortium: relationship to family history and conversion to psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(6):578-588. PubMed doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.66

47. Davidson M, Reichenberg A, Rabinowitz J, et al. Behavioral and intellectual markers for schizophrenia in apparently healthy male adolescents. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156(9):1328-1335. PubMed

48. Bilder RM, Reiter G, Bates J, et al. Cognitive development in schizophrenia: follow-back from the first episode. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2006;28(2):270-282. PubMed doi:10.1080/13803390500360554

49. Fuller R, Nopoulos P, Arndt S, et al. Longitudinal assessment of premorbid cognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia through examination of standardized scholastic test performance. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(7):1183-1189. PubMed doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.159.7.1183

50. Keefe RSE, Silva SG, Perkins DO, et al. The effects of atypical antipsychotic drugs on neurocognitive impairment in schizophrenia: a review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull. 1999;25(2):201-222. PubMed doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033374

51. Woodward ND, Purdon SE, Meltzer HY, et al. A meta-analysis of cognitive change with haloperidol in clinical trials of atypical antipsychotics: dose effects and comparison to practice effects. Schizophr Res. 2007;89(1-3):211-224. PubMed doi:10.1016/j.schres.2006.08.021

52. Davidson M, Galderisi S, Weiser M, et al. Cognitive effects of antipsychotic drugs in first-episode schizophrenia and schizophreniform disorder: a randomized, open-label clinical trial (EUFEST). Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166(6):675-682. PubMed doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08060806

53. Harvey PD, Keefe RS. Studies of cognitive change in patients with schizophrenia following novel antipsychotic treatment. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(2):176-184. PubMed doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.158.2.176

54. Keefe RS, Bilder RM, Davis SM, et al; Neurocognitive Working Group. Neurocognitive effects of antipsychotic medications in patients with chronic schizophrenia in the CATIE Trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(6):633-647. PubMed doi:10.1001/archpsyc.64.6.633

55. Keefe RS, Malhotra AK, Meltzer HY, et al. Efficacy and safety of donepezil in patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder: significant placebo/practice effects in a 12-week, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33(6):1217-1228. PubMed doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301499

Related Articles

Volume: 75

Quick Links: Autism Spectrum Disorders , Neurodevelopmental